Speech and language are uniquely human-specific traits, which contributed to humans becoming the predominant species on earth. Disruptions in the human speech and language function may result in diverse disorders. These include stuttering, aphasia, articulation disorder, spasmodic dysphonia, verbal dyspraxia, dyslexia and specific language impairment. Among these disorders, stuttering is the most common speech disorder characterized by disruptions in the normal flow of speech. Twin, adoption, and family studies have suggested that genetic factors are involved in susceptibility to stuttering. For several decades, multiple genetic studies including linkage analysis were performed to connect causative gene to stuttering, and several genetic studies have revealed the association of specific gene mutation with stuttering. One notable genetic discovery came from the genetic studies in the consanguineous Pakistani families. These studies suggested that mutations in the lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway genes (
Communication between human beings, mediated mostly by speech and language is a remarkable function that has had a significant influence on human life. Although speech and language are not easily distinguished, they are different from each other [1]. Speech is defined as the mechanical aspect of vocalization for communication, and includes articulation, voice and fluency. In contrast, language consists of non-mechanical syntactic rules such as grammar regulating the meaning of words, creation of new words, and application of words into meaningful combination to form a sentence (http://www.asha.org/public/speech/development/language_speech.htm). Both speech and language are regarded as unique features in humans that are not found in other species. Although it is well-known that many other animals can also communicate by generating vocalization, it is not entirely clear whether it has the syntactic rules. Human speech and language may be the most complex and well-organized form of vocalization compared with those in other animals [2,3].
When individuals have speech deficits, they are regarded as being affected with speech disorders, and these include fluency disorders (e.g., stuttering), voice disorder (e.g., spasmodic dysphonia), articulation disorder, verbal dyspraxia and aphasia [4-6]. Among these speech disorders, stuttering is the most common speech or fluency disorder characterized by repetitions, prolongations, and interruptions in the smooth flow of the speech [4]. In the population, stuttering shows high spontaneous recovery rate, with higher occurrence rate in males than female subjects [7,8]. Similarly, singing and speaking in unison mitigate symptoms. These puzzling clinical features, and the fact that stuttering has its origin in the brain, hinder researchers from investigating the neural basis of stuttering. However, several genetic studies such as twin, adoption, and segregation studies have suggested that this disorder occurs by the inheritance of gene mutations [9-16]. Genetic evidence presented by several approaches, in particular, genome-wide linkage analysis have shed light on these causative genes.
Here, we describe outcomes from the past, current, and future challenges encountered in the genetic studies of speech and language disorders.
Stuttering occur mostly in childhood. It affects up to 5% of the population at the age of 3-4 years, with the male-to-female ratio at 2:1 at the preschool age, and changes to 4:1 at the age 9. This sex-ratio change is due to that majority of the stutters (>75%) resolve spontaneously, especially in females [7,8]. Therefore, the estimated prevalence of stuttering is about 1% in the general population.
Consistent evidence for the involvement of genetic factors in stuttering has motivated studies aimed at identifying causative genetic mutations that could reveal underlying molecular and cellular deficits in this disorder. For example, twin studies revealed that concordance rate in monozygotic twins (63%) was higher than that in the dizygotic twins (9%) [9]. Half of the stutterers had a family history, and risk to first degree relative was 15%. Several large families with multiple individuals were affected by stuttering [10-15].
In addition, there was debate that stuttering can be learned rather than inherited from their parents. Felsenfeld and Plomin [16] designed adoption study to disentangle nature and nurture in 156 adopted and non-adopted children, and estimated their risk for stuttering based on parental history of stuttering. It was revealed that children with biological parents who stuttered showed 2.8 times higher risk for developing persistent stuttering than those with no parental history of stuttering. Thereby, implying that stuttering is not a learned behavior.
Based on a previous twin, adoption study, and the fact that stuttering runs in the family, it was strongly suggested that genetics may play an important role in the degree of susceptibility to stuttering. This motivated geneticists to perform genome-wide linkage analysis with the purpose to identify causative genes. Identifying the mode of inheritance and potential number of causative genes (monogenic or polygenic) were prerequisites for successful study of stuttering. Cox et al. [17] performed segregation analysis in 386 stutterers and their first-degree relatives to investigate precise model of transmission, and reported that genetic causes of stuttering cannot be attributed to single major locus, nor follow typical Mendelian mode of inheritance. Although there is substantial evidence that genetic factors are involved in susceptibility to stuttering, the suggested genetic model was found to be inconclusive.
Despite the unclear mode of inheritance, multiple genome-wide linkage studies were performed in an effort to identify chromosomal loci or genes in families affected by stuttering. The first linkage scan for stuttering was carried out by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD) at the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Shugart et al. [18] recruited 68 stuttering families with European Ancestry, and diagnosed affected individuals by recording both their conversation and reading skills [18]. Whole-genome scan using 392 microsatellite markers analyzed by GENEHUNTER and ALLEGRO programs found suggestive linkage (non-parametric linkage [NPL] score: 1.51) at D18S976 on chromosome 18p. Subsequent analysis with the single largest family alone revealed an NPL score of 5.35, which suggested that chromosome 18p is a predisposing locus for stuttering. Although this was the first whole-genome linkage analysis, further analysis to identify the causative gene in this locus remains to be performed.
Other genetic linkage studies by NIDCD/NIH applied to a single large Cameroonian family comprising 71 individuals, and to a group of 43 Brazilian families, reported genome-wide significant linkage of stuttering to the markers on chromosomes 2p, 15q (Cameroonian family: logarithm of odds [LOD] score=4.69?6.57) and 10q21 (Brazilian family, LOD score=4.28).
In addition, another research team at The Illinois International Genetics of Stuttering Project, University of Chicago also performed several whole-genome linkage analyses to identify genetic causes of stuttering. They recruited 100 families including 252 individuals of European ancestry, and found moderate linkages of stuttering to the microsatellite markers on multiple chromosomes 2, 7, 9, 15, and 21. Interestingly, sex-specific significant linkage scores were also reported in this study. In the analysis with male subjects only, linkage was found on markers at chromosome 7 (LOD score=2.99) while female-only analysis revealed linkage at chromosome 21 (LOD score=4.5). This sex-specific linkage results met the criteria for genome-wide significance [18]. Another interesting linkage study performed in a Hutterite population characterized by presence of relationship in a single genealogy with 232 individuals sharing the same ancestor, reported modest evidence of linkage on chromosomes 2, 3, 5, 13, and 15 [19].
Although tremendous effort was put into these linkage and association studies, they were limited in that they only reported chromosomal loci, and were unable to find any particular candidate genes for stuttering. Targeted deep-sequencing using next-generation sequencing on these linkage intervals need to be performed to investigate causative mutations in the near future.
All genome-wide linkage studies described above were in families from outbred populations until NIDCD/NIH group led by Drayna et al. [13] studied Pakistani inbred families affected by stuttering. In genetic studies of rare Mendelian disorders, consanguineous families were regarded as a promising starting point for success. Using a Pakistani family in genetic studies is advantageous because ~70% of all marriages are between either 1st or 2nd cousins. Consanguineous marriages result in a population structure with greatly increased incidence of recessive genetic disorders. This might be true for complex traits such as stuttering.
The first genome-wide linkage study of stuttering in Pakistani inbred family was performed by Riaz et al. [13] at NIDCD/NIH. In this study, forty-four families with multiple individuals affected by stuttering were ascertained from the city of Lahore and nearby areas in Pakistan. The status of stutterers was diagnosed using the Stuttering Severity Instrument, 3rd edition (SSI-3). Genome-wide linkage scan using microsatellite marker panel found significant linkage of stuttering to the markers at chromosome 12q23.3 (LOD score=4.61), which suggested that this significant linkage was mostly attributed to the largest family named PKST72 [13]. The linkage interval at chromosome 12q23.3 expands 10 megabases with PAH marker at the center, and eighty seven genes reside in this locus.
In an effort to follow-up on the linkage study of Pakistani inbred family, Kang et al. [14] performed comparative genomic hybridization to exclude possible presence of large insertional or deleterious (in/del) mutation in the affected individuals in this family. They concluded that there was no evidence of affected members carrying any large in/del mutation co-segregating with stuttering. Sanger sequencing of genes in this linkage interval revealed a missense mutation replacing glutamate with lysine residue at position 1,200 (p.Glu1,200Lys) in GlcNAc-1-phosphotransferase α/β, encoded by
Two other proteins, GlcNAc-1-phosphotransferase γ (encoded by
GlcNAc-1-phosphotransferase (EC2.7.8.17) is a hexameric complex consisting of three different subunits (2 alpha, 2 beta and 2 gamma subunits). Alpha and beta subunits are encoded by
Another question to be addressed includes why individuals with mutations in
GlcNAc-1-phosphodiester-N-acetylglucosaminidase (uncovering enzyme; EC 3.1.4.45), encoded by
The mutant NAGPA was not properly folded and localized in the ER rather than Golgi where normal NAGPA are mostly localized. Furthermore, this misfolded NAGPA was sensitive to degradation by proteasomal systems in the ER [28].
While data on GlcNAc-1-phosphotransferase remains limited due to the absence of functional assays, studies on the NAGPA enzyme showed that mutation reduced its enzymatic activity by about half. This resulted in disruptions in intracellular trafficking that led to a reduced half-life of the enzyme. However, it is still unclear how disruption in the lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway affects the speech function of the brain, thereby causing stuttering.
Recent genetic studies of stuttering within families and sporadic cases revealed multiple chromosomal loci linked with this disorder. Linkage analysis, particularly in the Pakistani inbred families revealed that mild disruption in the lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway mediated by
Another challenge is the development of suitable animal model system for stuttering studies. In the pipeline of genetic discovery, phenotype should be observed in the animal model when mutations found in human patients are introduced into the genome of the animal. However, human speech and language are human-specific traits, thus, stuttering phenotype is not observable in the animal model. Despite this uncertainty, the mouse model is known for communicating ultrasonic vocalization. Analysis and comparison of vocalization patterns of wild-type to knock-in/out mouse might be a potent approach to study the association of lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway and stuttering.
This study was supported by the intramural grant from Sungshin Women’s University (2013-1-11-062/1).
Lysosomal enzyme targeting pathway. Adding mannose-6-phosphate tag is mediated by two step procedures mediated by two enzymes, GlcNAc-1-phosphotransferase (encoded by